This is the first of a two-part post on the recent Neuron paper
Embodied information processing: vibrissa mechanics and texture features shape micromotions in actively sensing rats by Jason
Ritt and others in Christopher Moore's group. They provide a much-needed high-resolution look at the the movement of whiskers in freely-moving rats as they perform a discrimination task.
Where's the neuroscience?Before describing the main results, I'd like to discuss why, as
neuroscientists, we should care about what whiskers are doing. "Where is the neuroscience?" someone may ask of a paper that just examines whisker movements. To that I would respond, "What is the stimulus?" The first rule of
psychophysics and sensory coding is, "Know thy stimulus" (it was
EJ Chichilnisky that hammered this into my mind). Students of
psychophysics and sensory coding try to quantify the relationship between the stimulus and some other variable (either behavior (
psychophysics) or neuronal activity (sensory coding)), so we need to precisely quantify the stimulus on each trial to make sure we have all the relevant information contained in the stimulus.
Because of such concerns, the question we should be asking is why researchers studying coding in the whisker system know so
little about what the whiskers are doing, given the importance of understanding the stimulus in sensory coding and
psychophysics. In the present paper,
Ritt et al. provide a much-needed corrective to this deficit.
The resonance hypothesisFor a few years now, Moore (and
Mitra Hartmann at Northwestern) have been pointing out that whiskers are not simple passive detectors of what goes on in the world. Rather, whiskers have interesting intrinsic properties that shape their responses as the rat scurries about palpating the world.
For example, each whisker has a resonance frequency. If you attach a stimulator to the end of a whisker and move it at frequencies, there is a 'hot spot' at which the whisker will vibrate with a significantly higher amplitude. The following figure (Figure 2 from
Niemark et al) shows the amplitude of a whisker's movement as a function of stimulation frequency. The breakaway images show data from individual frequencies (gray line: stimulus, black line: whisker movement downstream from the stimulus):

This whisker in the figure has a resonance frequency just under 400 Hz. The resonance frequency of a whisker depends on that whisker's length (longer whiskers have lower resonance frequencies). Interestingly, the length of whiskers increases quite significantly as you move from the front to the back of a rat's face.
As an interesting historical aside, the
possiblity that whiskers have resonance frequencies was initially noticed by
Niemark while she was still an undergraduate working with John
Hopfield. Then the idea was fleshed out experimentally by
Niemark, Moore and Mark
Andermann (leading to
this paper) and by
Mitra Hartmann and others at Cal Tech (leading to
this paper). The present paper is the latest in a series of papers from Christopher Moore's group exploring the consequences of whisker resonance.
Getting back to the science, what is the significance of all this resonance business? Is it possible that whiskers with different resonant frequencies are induced to
resonate by different types of stimuli? This is part of what has become known as the
resonance hypothesis (
review here). As described in the original paper by
Niemark and others, "
vibrissa resonance is optimally positioned to increase the range of detection and the specificity of discrimination, and may provide the ability to represent complex stimuli through a compact,
somatotopically distributed code" (p 6500).
Many of us think of the resonance hypothesis as the 'whiskers as cochlea hairs' hypothesis. It has been a quite productive idea for experimentalists, and it is interesting to note that it wasn't even on our radar until
Neimark asked a simple question: what are the properties of the sensor used by the rat?
Resonant oscillations have been observed in detached whiskers (image above) and in the whiskers of anesthetized rats. It has also been shown that cortical neurons respond differently when a whisker is stimulated at its resonance frequency (see the above review of the resonance hypothesis). But is whisker resonance
ethologically important?
Does resonance really matter? Why it is hard to answer this questionA key question is whether resonance is important in the awake freely moving rat performing a discrimination task. One step toward
anwering this question is to determine whether resonance is observed in the whiskers in a particular task. If not, then resonance cannot be used so we'd have evidence that resonance is irrelevant for that task.
So, what's the problem? Measure the whiskers in awake freely moving rats and get on with it. Unfortunately, it turns out to be
extremely difficult to track tiny whisker movements in a rat that is running around moving its body, head, and whiskers any old place it wants. You need to get the optics just right to see many of these wispy whiskers in focus, use a high-speed camera (the resonance frequencies can be higher than 500 Hz), and then set up a system to automate the tracking of the whiskers (no way you are going to code all that data by hand with the required large
fields of view and fast frame rates). It would be a herculean effort.
Mitra Hartmann, presently at Northwestern, chipped away at this problem a bit. She analyzed some interesting video data from the awake freely moving rat
in this paper where whisker movements were sampled between 250 and 1000 Hz. She observed that whiskers often exhibit resonant oscillations after contacting an object, but not when the rat is simply whisking in open air.
But what about when a rat is performing a stimulus discrimination task? Could resonant oscillations actually be used to discriminate the features of an object such as its texture? To get at such questions,
Ritt et al have pushed things much further. They have developed algorithms to automatically track the entire length of individual whiskers during a texture discrimination task in freely moving rats. They capture images at the impressive frame rate of 3000 Hz. Here's one example from Figure 3 of their paper:

Here they show the results when a single whisker is tracked (it shows the shape of the whisker over multiple frames into the past in red) as the rat moves its head across a surface that changes from rough to smooth texture (you can think of it as moving from rough to smooth sandpaper). To someone interested in sensory coding and
psychophysics in the whisker system, this image is simply a symphony.
So what did they observe? To give away the punchline, it seems resonance is present during the task. But there is enough cool stuff in the paper that I'll put off the discussion of the results and interesting open questions to Part 2.
Acknowledgements Many thanks to
Mitra Hartmann and Christopher Moore for explaining some of the history of the resonance hypothesis and for clarification of some key points.